Page 53 - Summer2017
P. 53
culture has led to the development of complex knowledge and technology, building from generation to generation over time. We now know that whale culture can involve thousands of animals within a cultural group, with traditions spreading over thousands of kilometers and developing over decades or more, increasing in diversity over time. These cultural traditions involve communication, echolocation, and forag- ing behaviors, but we have little evidence for an increase in complexity for the short time periods over which they have been observed. There is little evidence for marine mammal culture involving technology; rather, it is social behaviors that appear to be especially important.
When you listen to sperm whale codas, they are not acousti- cally as striking and complex as killer whale calls or as mu- sical as humpback song (although they certainly are rhyth- mic). But when you think about codas as a cultural tradition, I hope that you agree that a relatively simple acoustic signal can actually represent a complex and fascinating communi- cation tradition.
Conclusion
Marine mammals brought mammalian sound production and hearing capacities to life in the ocean, which enabled them to exploit the favorable physics of sound in the sea. This enabled them to find prey and orient in the dark using echolocation and to communicate over ocean basin scales. Some marine mammals evolved remarkably sophisticated abilities to learn vocalizations, modifying the sounds they produce based on vocalizations they hear from conspecifics. This led to the development of fascinating animal cultures whose complexity we are only just beginning to understand.
Biosketch
Peter Tyack is professor of marine mammal biology at the University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Scotland, UK. He is a behavioral ecologist who stud- ies acoustic communication, vocal pro- duction learning, and social behavior in marine mammals, including reproduc-
tive advertisement in baleen whales, individually distinctive contact calls in delphinids, and echolocation in deep-diving toothed whales. In collaboration with engineer Mark John- son, he has developed sound-and-orientation recording tags. He has developed a series of studies on the responses
of marine mammals to anthropogenic sounds, including the effects of naval sonar and oil exploration on baleen and toothed whales.
References
Arnason, U., Gullberg, A., and Janke, A. (2004). Mitogenomic analyses pro- vide new insights into cetacean origin and evolution. Gene 333, 27-34.
Au, W. W. L. (2015). History of dolphin biosonar research. Acoustics Today 11, 10-17.
Bernard, A., Lécuyer, C., Vincent, P., Amiot, R., Bardet, N., Buffetaut, E., Cuny, G., Fourel, F., Martineau, F., Mazin, J. M., and Prieur, A. (2010). Regulation of body temperature by some Mesozoic marine reptiles. Sci- ence 328, 1379-1382.
Cerchio, S., Jacobsen, J. K., and Norris, T. F. (2001). Temporal and geo- graphical variation in songs of humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangli- ae: Synchronous change in Hawaiian and Mexican breeding assemblages. Animal Behaviour 62, 313-329.
Deecke, V. B., Ford, J. K., and Spong, P. (2000). Dialect change in resident killer whales: Implications for vocal learning and cultural transmission. Animal Behaviour 60, 629-638.
Galambos, R. (1942). The avoidance of obstacles by flying bats: Spallanzani’s ideas (1794) and later theories. Isis 34, 132-140.
Garland, E. C., Goldizen, A. W., Rekdahl, M. L., Constantine, R., Garrigue, C., Hauser, N. D., Poole, M. M., Robbins, J., and Noad, M. J. (2011). Dy- namic horizontal cultural transmission of humpback whale song at the ocean basin scale. Current Biology 21, 687-691.
Griffin, D. R. (1958). Listening in the Dark: The Acoustic Orientation of Bats and Men. Yale University Press, New Haven, CT.
Higdon, J. W., Bininda-Emonds, O. R. P., Beck, R. M. D., and Ferguson, S. H. (2007). Phylogeny and divergence of the pinnipeds (Carnivora: Mam- malia) assessed using a mutigene dataset. BMC Evolutionary Biology 7, 216.
Janik, V. M., and Sayigh, L. S. (2013). Communication in bottlenose dol- phins: 50 years of signature whistle research. Journal of Comparative Physi- ology A 199, 479-489.
Kasuya, T., and Marsh, H. (1984). Life history and reproductive biology of the short-finned pilot whale, Globicephala macrorhynchus, off the Pa- cific coast of Japan. In Perrin, W. F., Brownell, R. L., and DeMaster, D. P. (Eds.), Report of the International Whaling Commission. Special Issue 6, pp. 259-310.
King, S. L., Sayigh, L. S., Wells, R. S., Fellner, W., and Janik, V. M. (2013). Vocal copying of individually distinctive signature whistles in bottle- nosedolphins.ProceedingsoftheRoyalSocietyB:BiologicalSciences280, 20130053.
Madsen, P. T., Payne, R., Kristiansen, N. U., Wahlberg, M., Kerr, I., and Møhl, B. (2002). Sperm whale sound production studied with ultrasound time/depth-recording tags. Journal of Experimental Biology 205, 1899- 1906.
Madsen, P. T., Wilson, M., Johnson, M. P., Hanlon, R. T., Bocconcelli, A., Aguilar De Soto, N., and Tyack, P. L. (2007). Clicking for calamari: Toothed whales can echolocate squid Loligo pealeii. Aquatic Biology 1, 141-150.
Møhl, B., Wahlberg, M., Madsen, P. T., Heerfordt, A., and Lund, A. (2003). The monopulsed nature of sperm whale clicks. The Journal of the Acousti- cal Society of America 114, 1143-1154.
Nishimura, C. E. (1994). Monitoring whales and earthquakes by using SO- SUS. 1994 NRL Review. Naval Research Laboratory, Washington, DC, pp.
91-101.
Continued on page 64
Summer 2017 | Acoustics Today | 51