Page 44 - Spring2020
P. 44

Music Beyond Sound
Good, A., Reed, M. J., and Russo, F. A. (2014). Compensatory plastic- ity in the deaf brain: Effects on perception of music. Brain Sciences 4(4), 560-574.
Grahn, J. A. (2012). See what I hear? Beat perception in auditory and visual rhythms. Experimental Brain Research 220(1), 51-61. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3114-8.
Hyde, K. L., and Peretz, I. (2003). “Out-of-pitch” but still “in-time: An auditory psychophysical study in congenital amusic adults. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 999, 173-176.
Iversen, J. R., Patel, A. D., Nicodemus, B., and Emmorey, K. (2015). Synchronization to auditory and visual rhythms in hearing and deaf individuals. Cognition 134, 232-244.
Johnsrude, I. S., Penhune, V. B., and Zatorre, R. J. (2000). Functional speci- ficity in the right human auditory cortex for perceiving pitch direction. Brain 123(1), 155-163. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/123.1.155.
Kilner, J. M., Friston, K. J., and Frith, C. D. (2007). Predictive coding: An account of the mirror neuron system. Cognitive Processing 8(3), 159-166. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-007-0170-2.
Lahav, A., Saltzman, E., and Schlaug, G. (2007). Action representation of sound: Audiomotor recognition network while listening to newly acquired actions. The Journal of Neuroscience 27(2), 308-314. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4822-06.2007.
Lauzon, A., Russo, F. A., and Harris, L. (2020). The influence of rhythm on detection of auditory and vibrotactile asynchronies. Experimental Brain Research, in press.
London, J. (2012). Hearing in Time: Psychological Aspects of Musical Meter. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.
Loui, P. (2015). A dual-Stream Neuroanatomy of Singing. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal 32(3), 232-241. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2015.32.3.232.
Loui, P. (2019). Music and the brain. Acoustics Today, 15(4), 29-36. https://doi.org/10.1121/AT.2019.15.4.29.
Makous, J. C., Friedman, R. M., and Vierck, C. J. (1995). A critical band filter in touch. The Journal of Neuroscience 15(4), 2808-2818. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-04-02808.1995.
Marshall, M. T., and Wanderley, M. M. (2011). Examining the effects of embedded vibrotactile feedback on the feel of a digital musical instrument. Proceedings of the International Conference on New Interfaces for Musical Expression, Oslo, Norway, May 30–June 1, 2011, pp. 399-404.
McGarry, L. M., Pineda, J. A., and Russo, F. A. (2015). The role of the extended MNS in emotional and nonemotional judgments of human song. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 15(1), 32-44. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-014-0311-x.
Milner, B. (1962). Laterality effects in Audition. In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Inter- hemispheric Relations and Cerebral Dominance. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD, pp. 177-195.
Moore, D. (2019). Living in a world without sound. The Globe and Mail, November 14, 2019. https://tgam.ca/2KN2lwl.
Morioka, M., and Griffin, M. J. (2005). Thresholds for the perception of hand-transmitted vibration: Dependence on contact area and contact location. Somatosensory and Motor Research 22(4), 281-297. https://doi.org/10.1080/08990220500420400.
Müller, K., Aschersleben, G., Schmitz, F., Schnitzler, A., Freund, H. J., and Prinz, W. (2008). Inter-versus intramodal integration in sensorimotor synchronization: A combined behavioral and magnetoencephalographic study. Experimental Brain Research 185(2), 309-318. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1155-1.
Obleser, J., Henry, M. J., and Lakatos, P. (2017). What do we talk about when we talk about rhythm?. PLoS Biology 15(9), e2002794.
Papetti, S., Jarvelainen, H., Giordano, B. L., Schiesser, S., and Frohlich, M. (2017). Vibrotactile sensitivity in active touch: Effect of pressing force. IEEE Transactions on Haptics 10(1), 113-122. https://doi.org/10.1109/TOH.2016.2582485.
Patel, A. D., Iversen, J. R., Chen, Y., and Repp, B. H. (2005). The influence of metricality and modality on synchronization with a beat. Experimental Brain Research 163(2), 226-238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-2159-8.
Peretz, I. (1990). Processing of local and global musical information by unilateral brain-damaged patients. Brain 113(4), 1185-1205. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/113.4.1185.
Phillips-Silver, J., and Trainor, L. J. (2005). Feeling the beat: Movement influences infant rhythm perception. Science 308, 1430. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1110922.
Phillips-Silver, J., and Trainor, L. J. (2007). Hearing what the body feels: Auditory encoding of rhythmic movement. Cognition 105(3), 533-546.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.11.006.
Rosenblum, L. D. (2019). Does sensory modality matter? Not for speech perception. Acoustics Today 15(3), 47-54. https://doi.org/10.1121/AT.2019.15.3.47.
Rosenblum, L. D., and Fowler, C. A. (1991). Audiovisual investigation of the loudness-effort effect for speech and nonspeech events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 17(4), 976- 985. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.17.4.976.
Russo, F. A. (2019). Multisensory processing in music. In M. Thaut and D. Hodges (Eds.), Handbook of Music and the Brain. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.
Russo, F. A., Ammirante, P., and Fels, D. I. (2012). Vibrotactile discrimination of musical timbre. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 32, 822-826. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029046.
Russo, F. A., Sandstrom, G. M., and Maksimowski, M. (2011). Mouth versus eyes: Gaze fixation during perception of sung interval size. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain 21(1), 98-107. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0094007.
Saldaña, H. M., and Rosenblum, L. D. (1993). Visual influences on auditory pluck and bow judgments. Perception & Psychophysics 54(3), 406-416.
Schürmann, M., Caetano, G., Hlushchuk, Y., Jousmäki, V., and Hari, R. (2006). Touch activates human auditory cortex. NeuroImage 30(4), 1325-1331.
Schutz, M. (2008). Seeing music? What musicians need to know about vision. Empirical Musicology Review 3(3), 83-108.
Schutz, M., and Kubovy, M. (2009). Deconstructing a musical illusion: Point-light representations capture salient properties of impact motions. Canadian Acoustics 37, 23-28.
Schutz, M., and Lipscomb, S. (2007). Hearing gestures, seeing music: Vision influences perceived tone duration. Perception 36(6), 888-897. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5635.
Thompson, W. F., and Russo, F. A. (2007). Facing the music. Psychological Science 18(9), 756-757.
Thompson, W., Russo, F., and Livingstone, S. (2010). Facial expressions of singers influence perceived pitch relations. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 17(3), 317-322. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.3.317.
Tranchant, P., Shiell, M. M., Giordano, M., Nadeau, A., Peretz, I., and Zatorre, R. J. (2017). Feeling the beat: Bouncing synchronization to vibro- tactile music in hearing and early deaf people. Frontiers in Neuroscience 11, Article 507. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2017.00507.
Verrillo, R. T. (1992). Vibration Sensation in Humans. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal 9(3), 281-302. https://doi.org/10.2307/40285553.
von Békésy, G., and Wever, E. G. (1960). Experiments in Hearing. McGraw-Hill, New York
44 | Acoustics Today | Spring 2020

























































   42   43   44   45   46