Page 50 - Volume 12, Issue 2 - Spring 2012
P. 50

  Fig. 7. Direction of acoustic-motion discrimination (mean performance from four cats) performance in the left hemifield, right hemifield, and positions that traverse the midline (both hemifields) prior to, and following, cooling deactivation (Pre/Post, red), during bilateral cooling of the anterior auditory field (AAF) (dark blue), during bilat- eral cooling of the primary auditory field (A1) (light blue), and during bilateral cooling of posterior auditory field (PAF) (green). Single asterisk indicates performance sig- nificantly different (p<0.01) from pre/post cooling levels, and double asterisks indicate performance significantly different (p<0.01) from both pre/post cooling levels and from bilateral A1 cooling deactivation performance. Dashed line=chance performance of 50% correct.
sequential noise bursts (78 dB SPL) emitted from five speak- ers across 60° of arc. The cat then made a correct response by approaching a left or right 30° LED to indicate leftward or rightward motion, respectively. Both rightward and leftward motion were examined in three different zones of the audito- ry field: positions confined to the left hemifield; positions that crossed the midline; and positions that were confined to the left hemifield (Fig. 7). Performance prior to, and follow- ing, cooling deactivation was in excess of 80% correct. During bilateral deactivation of AAF cortex, discrimination of acoustic-motion direction was unimpaired and not signif- icantly different from control levels (Fig. 7). Bilateral deacti- vation (i.e., cooling) of A1 resulted in an acoustic-motion discrimination impairment throughout the entire field exam- ined, with performance falling to ~65% correct (p<0.01; Fig. 7). In contrast, bilateral deactivation of PAF performance fell to chance levels during bilateral cooling of PAF. Therefore, as with the static-localization task, deficits were most profound during bilateral deactivation of PAF, moderate during bilater- al deactivation of A1, and absent during bilateral AAF deac- tivation.
Overall, the results show that areas involved in acoustic- motion processing are also involved in static acoustic spatial localization. An area that is uniquely involved in acoustic- motion processing was not identified. These results suggest that spatial-localization functions may be a prerequisite for acoustic-motion processing in auditory cortex, and do not support the possibility of the existence of an auditory equiv- alent of the visual field MT.AT
References
Aston-Jones, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2005). “An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance,” Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 403–450.
Bandyopadhyay, S., Shamma, S. A., and Kanold, P. O. (2010). “Dichotomy of functional organization in the mouse auditory cortex,” Nature Neurosci. 13, 361–368.
Born, R. T., and Bradley, D. C. (2005). “Structure and function of visual area MT,” Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 157–189.
Carcea, I., Yuan, K., Barker, A., Seybold, S., Martins, A. R., Zaika, N., Bernstein, H., Polley, D., Merzenich, M. M., Schreiner, C. E., and Froemke, R. C. (2012). “Long-term modification of cortical synapses improves sensory perception,” 9th Cosyne, Abstract T- 27, pp. 36–37, Salt Lake City.
Chen, X., Leischner, U., Rochefort, N. L., Nelken, I., and Konnerth, A. (2011). “Functional mapping of single spines in cortical neu- rons in vivo,” Nature 475, 501–505.
Fritz, J. B., David, S. V., Radtke-Schuller, S., Yin, P., and Shamma, S. A. (2010). “Adaptive, behaviorally gated, persistent encoding of task-relevant auditory information in ferret frontal cortex,” Nature Neurosci. 13, 1011–1019.
Fritz, J. B., Elhilali, M., and Shamma, S. A. (2005a). “Differential dynamic plasticity of A1 receptive fields during multiple spectral tasks,” J. Neurosci. 25, 7623–7635.
Fritz, J. B., Elhilali, M., and Shamma, S. (2005b). “Active listening: Task-dependent plasticity of spectrotemporal receptive fields in primary auditory cortex,” Hearing Res. 206, 159–176.
Froemke, R. C., Merzenich, M. M., and Schreiner, C. E. (2007). “A synaptic memory trace for cortical receptive field plasticity,” Nature 450, 425–429.
Hubel, D. H., Henson, C. O., Rupert, A., and Galambos, R. (1959). “Attention units in the auditory cortex,” Science 129, 1279–1280.
Lomber, S. G., and Malhotra, S. (2008). “Double dissociation of “what” and “where” processing in auditory cortex,” Nature Neurosci. 11, 609–616.
Lomber, S. G., Malhotra, S., and Hall, A. J. (2007). “Functional spe- cialization in non-primary auditory cortex of the cat: Areal and laminar contributions to sound localization,” Hearing Res. 229, 31–45.
Malhotra, S., Stecker, G. C., Middlebrooks, J. C., and Lomber, S. G. (2008). “Sound localization deficits during reversible deactiva- tion of primary auditory cortex and/or the dorsal zone,” J. Neurophysiol. 99, 1628–1642.
Malhotra, S., and Lomber, S. G. (2007). “Sound localization during homotopic and heterotopic bilateral cooling deactivation of pri- mary and non-primary auditory cortical areas in the cat,” J. Neurophysiol. 97, 26–43.
Ojima, H., Taoka, M., and Iriki, A. (2010). “Adaptive changes in fir- ing of primary auditory cortical neurons following illumination shift from light to dark in freely moving guinea pigs,” Cerebral Cortex 20, 339–351.
Read, H. L., Winer, J. A., and Schreiner, C. E. (2002). “Functional architecture of auditory cortex. Current Opinions Neurobiol. 12, 433–440.
Rothschild, G., Nelken, I., and Mizrahi, A. (2010). “Functional organization and population dynamics in the mouse primary auditory cortex,” Nature Neurosci. 13, 353–360.
Seligman, M. E., and Beagley, G. (1975). “Learned helplessness in the rat,” J. Comparative and Physiol. Psychol. 88, 534–541.
Tan, A. Y., and Wehr, M. (2009). “Balanced tone-evoked synaptic Auditory Cortical Function 49












































































   48   49   50   51   52