Page 50 - Spring 2018
P. 50

Anthropocene Soundscape Ecology
ronment. There are also reports of matches between a gap in the ambient-noise spectrum and the sound spectrum used by fish for communicating to mates and competitors. Many taxa also exhibit flexible coping strategies for fluctuating ambient-noise levels. Tamarin monkeys (Saguinus oedipus), for example, avoid noisy periods and call preferentially in windows of relative quiet, whereas some bat species raise the amplitude of their echolocation calls in response to a rise in ambient-sound levels (Brumm and Slabbekoorn, 2005).
The widespread importance of sounds taxonomically and the critical role of the acoustic environment in their func- tionality and evolution means that changes to the sound- scapes in the Anthropocene may also have widespread con- sequences. Conservation is, in that respect, an odd term because almost all ecosystems are dynamic and changes are inherent to nature. We should, therefore, not necessarily aim to stop all changes. Furthermore, we have also seen that many species are able to cope with flexible responses in cases of novel environmental factors such as elevating noise levels. However, we often lack insight into the energetic costs of ad- justing signals and whether or not there are positive fitness consequences for the animals. As a result, the link between flexibility and breeding success is not yet confirmed and may turn out to be just a temporary solution for the individual. Persistence of the species in an increasingly noisy environ- ment is therefore by no means guaranteed.
The best advice at the moment should therefore be: do not make sounds when it is not essential. Many of our activities only produce sound as a by-product of their function. Noise mitigation is, therefore, often a reasonable solution. Another sensible thing is to make use of soundscapes for monitor- ing, as discussed in an article in this issue of Acoustics Today by Miksis-Olds, Martin, and Tyack. Recordings tell more than a thousand pictures and not only report which species are present but potentially also whether they are breeding, whether they were able to get a mate, what condition they are in, and whether they have an acoustic strategy to cope flexibly with fluctuating noise conditions. Changes over time in the presence and relative prominence of anthropo- genic noise can be quantified and matched with alterations in the soundscape spectrum due to changes in the vocal ani- mal community. Hopefully, more recordings and more stud- ies will improve both our understanding and appreciation for the acoustic world around us and make such efforts not just for the record.
Acknowledgments
Lucia Berti created all the artwork for the figures. Arthur Popper and Micheal Dent reviewed an earlier draft of the manuscript.
References
Barber, J. R., Crooks, K. R., and Fristrup, K. M. (2010). The costs of chronic noise exposure for terrestrial organisms. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 25, 180-189.
Brooks, B. M., Schulte-Fortkamp, B., Voigt, K. S., and Case, A. U. (2014). Exploring our sonic environment through soundscape research and theo- ry. Acoustics Today 10(1), 30-40.
Brumm, H. (2004). The impact of environmental noise on song amplitude in a territorial bird. Journal of Animal Ecology 73, 434-440.
Brumm, H., and Slabbekoorn, H. (2005). Acoustic communication in noise. Advances in the Study of Behavior 35, 151-209.
Brumm, H., and Zollinger, S. A. (2011). The evolution of the Lombard effect: 100 years of psychoacoustic research. Behaviour 148(11-13), 1173-1198.
Carson, R. (1962). Silent Spring. Houghton Mifflin, Boston.
Dingle, C., Halfwerk, W., and Slabbekoorn, H. (2008). Habitat-dependent song divergence at subspecies level in the grey-breasted wood-wren. Jour-
nal of Evolutionary Biology 21, 1079-1089.
Dominoni, D. M., Greif, S., Nemeth, E., and Brumm, H. (2016). Airport
noise predicts song timing of European birds. Ecology and Evolution 17,
6151-6159.
Endler, J. A. (1993). Some general comments on the evolution and design
of animal communication systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Society London B: Biological Sciences 340, 215-225.
Francis, C. D., and Barber, J. R. (2013). A framework for understanding
noise impacts on wildlife: An urgent conservation priority. Frontiers in
Ecology and the Environment 11, 305-313.
Francis, C. D., Ortega, C. P., and Cruz, A. (2009). Cumulative consequences
of noise pollution: Noise changes avian communities and species interac-
tions. Current Biology 19, 1415-1419.
Halfwerk, W., Bot, S., Buikx, J., van der Velde, M., Komdeur, J., ten Cate,
C., and Slabbekoorn, H., (2011). Low songs lose potency in urban noise conditions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108, 14549-14554.
Halfwerk, W., and Slabbekoorn, H. (2009). A behavioural mechanism ex- plaining noise-dependent frequency use in urban birdsong. Animal Be- haviour 78, 1301-1307.
Halfwerk, W., and Slabbekoorn, H. (2014). The impact of anthropogenic noise on avian communication and fitness. In Gil, D., and Brumm, H. (Eds.), Avian Urban Ecology: Physiological and Behavioural Adaptations. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, pp. 84-97.
Handel, S. (1989). Listening. An Introduction to the Perception of Auditory Events. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Klump, G. M. (1996). Bird communication in the noisy world. In Kroods- ma, D. E., and Miller, E. H. (Eds.), Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Com- munication in Birds. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY, pp. 321-338.
Krause, B. (2013). The voice of the natural world. Bernie Krause at TEDG- lobal 2013. Available at http://acousticstoday.org/bkrause.
LaZerte, S. E., Slabbekoorn, H., and Otter, K. A. (2016). Learning to cope: Vocal adjustment to urban noise is correlated with prior experience in black-capped chickadees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sci- ences 283, 20161058.
  48 | Acoustics Today | Spring 2018































































   48   49   50   51   52